Trail Making Test (TMT) Neuropsychological Test 

 

At the end of this guide you will be able to…

  • Know what the Trail Making Test measures and when to use it in your research
  • Run the Trail Making Test in Testable starting from our ready-made template
  • Customize key parameters to adapt this test to your needs
  • Collect data and interpret the results

If you never worked with Testable before, you can check out our 10-minute introduction video here and come back

Try the demo


What is the Trail Making Test and how you can use it in your own research

The Trail Making Test (TMT) originated with Ralph Reitan, who developed it as part of the Army Individual Test Battery (1944) to assess cognitive function in soldiers. Over time, it has become a widely adopted neuropsychological test in clinical and research settings due to its effectiveness in assessing cognitive flexibility, attention, and visual scanning abilities. (Wikipedia)

The TMT has been extensively used in clinical and research settings to assess cognitive flexibility in a wide range of populations, including individuals with traumatic brain injuries, neurodegenerative diseases, psychiatric disorders, and healthy aging.

In the TMT, participants go through two tasks, each containing a series of 25 stimuli. In the first task, known as Part A, participants must connect a sequence of numbered circles in ascending order as quickly as possible.This segment primarily assesses visual attention and processing speed.

Trail Making Test - part A

Part B introduces a more complex task. Participants must alternate between connecting numbered circles and lettered circles in ascending order (e.g., 1-A-2-B). This task requires participants to exhibit cognitive flexibility and the ability to maintain and switch between different mental sets.

Trail Making Test - part B

 

But how do we use the TMT to measure cognitive flexibility?

One of the most significant observations from the TMT is the presence of what’s known as “switch costs“. It represents the additional time and cognitive effort required to switch between tasks or mental sets, compared to when we’re performing the same task repeatedly.

When participants transition from Part A to Part B of the TMT, they often experience an increase in response times and error rates. This increase indicates the cognitive demands associated with task-switching. Essentially, participants need to disengage from the simple task of connecting numbered circles and engage in the more complex task of alternating between numbers and letters and keeping track of their sequences.

Switch costs are a hallmark of cognitive flexibility, reflecting the brain’s ability to adapt and reconfigure mental processes in response to changing task demands. Individuals with higher levels of cognitive flexibility typically exhibit smaller switch costs and are more efficienct in transitioning between mental sets.

Moreover, the magnitude of switch costs can vary depending on individual differences and contextual factors. For example, younger adults may demonstrate smaller switch costs compared to older adults, reflecting age-related changes in cognitive flexibility. Additionally, individuals with traumatic brain injuries or neurodegenerative diseases may show higher switch costs due to impaired executive functioning.

How to run the Trail Making Test in Testable

In the Testable version of the Trail Making Test, participants complete the full assessment in just two trials. Importantly, the circle placement mirrors the standard TMT format, ensuring consistency in administration and scoring methods.

At the start of the experiment, participants receive detailed instructions for both parts of the test and informed that they have a total of five minutes to complete both tasks.

Comparison between the original and computerised versions of the Trail Making Test

After completing both parts of the TMT, Testable automatically saves response times and errors. You can easily export this data and analyse it to assess the participant’s cognitive flexibility and processing speed.

Where to find the Trail Making Test in Testable

You can find the TMT template in our Library. It is ready to run. Import it to your account and start collecting data immediately or customize it first. You can fully customize experiments in Testable, and access them in all browsers.

How to customize key parameters and play around with different options

The trial file has all the information Testable needs, formatted in a simple spreadsheet. You can edit it using any spreadsheet editor like Google Sheets, Excel, or Testable’s built-in editor.

Here are two ways you can tailor the TMT experiment:

  1. Control the flow of your tasks: By default, the Testable TMT requires participants to submit the entire correct sequence before moving on to the next task. You can remove this restriction by deleting the ‘if’ and ‘then’ columns from your trial file, allowing participants to proceed even if their previous sequence was incorrect.
  2. Offering performance feedback: Testable provides feedback for incorrect responses and normative data comparisons based on the participant’s country (where available). If you don’t want to include this feedback or normative data, you can simply delete the relevant trials and columns from the trial file.

After making your changes, save and upload the modified trial file to your experiment’s trial file section.

Testable TMT setup
Testable Trail Making Test Setup

Collect data by sending the experiment link to your participants

Once you import this template to your library, collect data by sharing the unique experiment link (e.g., tstbl.co/xxx-xxx) with participants. Once participants complete the experiment, their results will appear in the ‘Results’ section of your experiment.

Working with results from the Trail Making Test

Testable automatically records participants’ response time (the RT” column in the results files) and accuracy (in the “correct column). If participants correctly connect all circles in the right order, the trial’s accuracy is marked with 1. If the sequence of responses is incorrect, the trial’s accuracy is marked with 0.

You also receive detailed response times for each key press during the trial (‘RT_keys‘ column) and the ‘timestamp’ for when each trial has ended.

Analysing the Trail Making Test results

 

When analyzing the results from the TMT, here are the key aspects to focus on:

  • Correct Trials: First, filter out the incorrect trials. Focus on rows where the correct column is 1. In our example, the participant submitted an incorrect sequence in the TMT part B (trial in row 7) and was promoted to repeat the trial. Therefore, the correct trials to focus on are trial_A (row 6) and the second attempt of trial_B (row 7_2).
  • Reaction Times (RT): You can find the achieved RT in the designated column. But if you want to work with values in seconds you can extract those from the tallies column (this column records the data our custom script ran). The achieved RT for each trial is recorded in the RT column. If you prefer working with values in seconds, you can extract these from the tallies column, which records the data from our custom script designed to convert RT to seconds for display purposes.
  • Error Analysis: While this example focuses on correct trials, analyzing incorrect trials can provide insights into common mistakes and areas where participants struggled. This can be particularly useful in clinical settings to identify specific cognitive deficits.
  • Individual Response Times (RTkeys): We can also examine the RTkeys column for detailed response times of each key press during the trial as longer times at specific points may indicate areas of difficulty or hesitation, for example.

Calculating the switch cost:

Now, the true power of the TMT lies in analyzing the difference between the RT for Part A and Part B. This difference, often called the “switch cost” reflects the participant’s cognitive flexibility and ability to shift between different cognitive demands. A larger difference in RT between Part A and Part B indicates difficulties in cognitive switching. These difficulties may be associated with various neurological or neuropsychological conditions.

Typically, response times on Part B are longer than on Part A because of the additional cognitive demands involved in task switching.

To determine the switch cost subtract RT in trial_A from RT in trial_B.

Visual representation of the switch cost in the Trail Making Test

Considerations for normative data comparison

We should compare individual performance to normative data tailored to factors like age, education, and cultural background. Therefore, if the study includes German participants, referring to normative data specific to the German population or similar cultural contexts can provide more accurate insights.

***

The Testable version of the TMT records RT in milliseconds and converts it to seconds for comparisons. The template includes custom code for this conversion and provides participants with performance feedback (see ‘script’ column).

After completing the test, participants can review their performance against normative data categorized by their country of origin, if available.